Back to articles
Regular Articles
Volume: 3 | Article ID: jpi0130
Image
Modeling the Relationship between Impressions and Image Features of Crinkle Finish of DSLR Camera
  DOI :  10.2352/J.Percept.Imaging.2020.3.2.020503  Published OnlineMarch 2020
Abstract
Abstract

In this article, the authors study the relationships between the principal impressions of crinkle-finished surfaces, which are found on camera exteriors, and the characteristics of test images of crinkle-finished surfaces. They extracted impression words for the surfaces through subjective experiments with humans. The results suggest that the impression is affected by five factors: two physical impression factors and three emotional impression factors. The surface images were obtained using a multi-angle measurement system that was built to collect images under various conditions. The authors used stepwise multiple regression to derive equations to predict the impressions of the surface given the characteristics of its test images. The results of the equations are highly correlated with the subjective scores of the five impression factors. These models will enable designers to design attractive crinkle-finished surfaces and camera exteriors.

Subject Areas :
Views 74
Downloads 16
 articleview.views 74
 articleview.downloads 16
  Cite this article 

Takayuki Gotoh, Takuroh Sone, Yusuke Tani, Kensuke Tobitani, Noriko Nagata, "Modeling the Relationship between Impressions and Image Features of Crinkle Finish of DSLR Camerain Journal of Perceptual Imaging,  2020,  pp 020503-1 - 020503-10,  https://doi.org/10.2352/J.Percept.Imaging.2020.3.2.020503

 Copy citation
  Copyright statement 
Copyright © Society for Imaging Science and Technology 2020
  Article timeline 
  • received October 2010
  • accepted July 2020
  • PublishedMarch 2020

Preprint submitted to:
jpi
Journal of Perceptual Imaging
J. Percept. Imaging
J. Percept. Imaging
2575-8144
Society for Imaging Science and Technology
1.
Introduction
Appearance is an important factor that affects the motivation of buyers of industrial products for consumers. Surface finish painting and fabrication methods to alter a product’s appearance have been developed for many years. In digital single-lens reflex (DSLR) cameras, the impression of a premium feeling is a key factor. Therefore, manufacturers have developed painting methods to generate this impression.
One painting method used to provide the feeling of premium quality on camera exteriors is crinkle finish. Figure 1 shows an example of a crinkle-finished surface, where dark particles are splashed onto a black base surface. These surfaces are mainly used for the exterior of DSLR and parts of automobiles; for example, head covers of engines. Designers determine the qualities of a crinkle finish to increase affective value. However, these qualities may result in unintended or even negative affective value because the crinkle finishes are determined by a designer’s subjective impressions, which may not correspond to those of the customer. To avoid such problems, it is important to clarify the principal impressions of a crinkle finish and construct a model to relate those impressions with surface features. This would make it possible to design a crinkle-finished surface for providing the intended impression.
Figure 1.
Exterior of a digital single-lens reflex camera and magnified image of a crinkle-finished surface.
Kansei engineering is a philosophy and method for product design, which considers what consumers feel from products and not only functions [10, 16, 20]. Regarding the relationships between impressions and shape design of products, some previous studies have analyzed car front grill design [7], PET container design of bottled beverages [1], cosmetic box design [12], and glass design [2]. With respect to the relationships between impressions and surface features, some previous studies have also analyzed spectral spatial features of pearl surfaces [14, 17, 19], exterior colors of cellular phones [9], image features of wood floor materials [13], and physical properties of woven fabrics [15]. In contrast, there exist no studies that extract and model the principal impressions for exterior paint on DSLRs.
The purpose of this study is two-fold: (1) to determine the factors that influence the visual impressions of a crinkle finish and (2) to model the relationships between these factors and image characteristics. These relationships can help designers to provide better impressions to users of DSLRs.
Hence, we conducted experiments to evaluate the impression of crinkle-finished samples and measured the features of images of the surfaces.
Previously, we had reported the results of impression evaluation and modeling the relationships between a selected list of the impressions and image characteristics [3]. In this article, we report the results of impression evaluation and modeling the relationships between all extracted impressions through these experiments and image characteristics of surfaces.
2.
Experiment 1
Impression evaluation experiments are psychological tests that clarify and quantify principal impressions of samples [18]. There is no standard dataset of words used to evaluate product impression for consumer electronic devices. Therefore, we had to create our own dataset. To determine the proper evaluation words, we conducted three experiments before the impression evaluation experiment: an impression-word extraction test, an impression-word appropriateness test, and a rating word distance measurement test. Through an impression-word extraction test, we gathered impressions of crinkle-finished surfaces widely. However, these widely gathered words contain uncommon or personal impressions. Therefore, we extracted users’ common expressions by an impression-word appropriateness test. Then we conducted a rating word distance test to group similar impressions.
2.1
Methods
2.1.1
Impression-Word Extraction Test
First, we extracted general impressions about crinkle finishes. Fourteen subjects observed three crinkle-finished samples and two general DSLRs and then wrote down impressions they felt with single words or short phrases as many as they could. They were encouraged to clamp samples and view them at various angles. They could write down only visual impressions, not haptic impressions. The subjects were encouraged to observe a small flat area (about 15 mm × 20 mm) to avoid the effects of sample and camera shapes. The subjects were image science engineers in our company and consisted of 13 men and 1 woman in their 20s to 50s. They were selected randomly without any consideration of their knowledge of DSLR. They were told that samples were parts of DSLR exteriors.
This test was conducted using a standard light source (Spectra Light QC; X-Rite, MA, USA). Standard illuminant D65 was used, and the illuminance of the observation area was set at 1,300 lx. These conditions were also used for the appropriateness tests and impression evaluation experiments.
After all participants wrote down impressions, we took all words that had appeared at least once. Then, 114 words were extracted from this experiment. Additionally, we added 25 words used by paint designers. A total of 139 words were extracted to describe the general impressions of crinkle finish.
2.1.2
Impression-Word Appropriateness Test
The words gathered by the impression-word extraction test contain complex and uncommon words. These words are not appropriate for use in evaluation experiments. Therefore, in this test, we evaluated appropriateness of these words for crinkle paint impression evaluation.
In this test, 21 participants evaluated the appropriateness of 139 words using seven scores from 1 (“very inappropriate”) to 7 (“very appropriate”). Subjects observed three crinkle-finished samples and two general DSLRs and then evaluated how appropriate each word was for the impression of samples. In this test, subjects could also clamp samples and observe them at various angles. Subjects were encouraged to observe a small flat area to avoid the effects of sample and camera shapes. Ten participants were instructed to evaluate how appropriate each word was for the impression of physical condition and 11 participants were instructed to evaluate how appropriate each word was for the impression of what they feel from samples. Because the number of evaluation words must be smaller than the number of samples to perform factor analyses, the number of evaluated impressions is limited. Therefore, we analyzed physical impressions and emotional impressions separately to evaluate more impressions [5, 6, 11, 21]. We call the former “low-level impressions” and the latter “high-level impressions.” We calculated the average and standard deviation of each sample score. Words with an average score of more than five and a standard deviation of less than 1.5 were selected as appropriate words.
We extracted 36 words appropriate for low-level impressions and 32 words appropriate for high-level impressions. Eighteen words were appropriate for both levels of impression.
As mentioned in Section 1, the crinkle finish is intended to provide a premium feeling. However, no words related to ‘feels premium’ were extracted by the impression-word appropriateness test. Hence, we added “feels premium” to the high-level impression appropriate words to investigate the relationship between the premium feeling and other impressions.
2.1.3
Rating Word Distance Measurement Test
We conducted a rating word distance measurement test to measure the distances of the meanings between impression words.
Participants were handed the list of all pairs of words extracted in the previous experiment and judged whether each pair of words could be exchanged semantically. This experiment was conducted without the observation of samples. Ten participants evaluated pairs of low-level impression words. They had also evaluated appropriateness of low-level impression at the impression-word appropriateness test. Twelve participants evaluated pairs of high-level impression words. Among 12 participants, 11 participants had also evaluated appropriateness of high-level impression at the impression-word appropriateness test and 1 was a new participant. The new participant was shown crinkle-finished samples before this experiment.
Then, the word distances between each pair were calculated by the following equation:
(1)
D=NnNall.
Here, D is the word distance between a pair of words, Nall is the number of all participants, and Nn is the number of participants who judged that the pair of words could not be exchanged semantically. When all participants answer that a pair of words cannot be exchanged, D = 1. When all participants answer that a pair of words can be exchanged, D = 0. Therefore, D corresponds to normalized similarity between a pair of appropriate words. A pair with a short distance indicates a similar impression and a pair with a long distance indicates different impressions.
We plotted the words in multi-dimensional space using multi-dimensional scaling. The maximum number of dimensions of the space is one less than the number of appropriate words. To analyze the similarities between appropriate words correctly, low- and high-level impression words were plotted in 35- and 31-dimensional spaces, respectively.
To group impression words according to the similarity of impressions, hierarchical cluster analysis was conducted using Ward’s method. The high- and low-level impression words were divided into nine clusters, each corresponding to one type of impression. The nine impression words that were the nearest to the center of mass of each cluster were selected as the final evaluation words (Table I).
Table I.
Evaluation words.
Low-level impressionsHigh-level impressions
smoothgranularslickfine
fineroughstatelynonuniform
nonuniformglitteringclearbumpy
cloudylumpylumpysimple
mattesparklingfeels premium
2.1.4
Impression Evaluation Experiment
Figure 2 shows an example of the crinkle paint samples used in the impression evaluation experiments. The samples are parts of camera exteriors that were finished using crinkle paints. Table II shows the quantity and size of the paint particles used for the 10 crinkle-finished samples. Three levels of each factor were tested. The sizes of large particles are approximately 0.08 mm2, medium particles are 0.06 mm2, and small particles are 0.05 mm2. Furthermore, we used two particle coating materials (TYPE-A and TYPE-B). The base coating material is the same for all samples. Figure 3 shows some examples of pictures of sample surfaces.
Figure 2.
Example of crinkle paint (left) and magnified image of the crinkle-finished surface (right).
Table II.
Crinkle-finish sample types.
Coating material TYPE-A
Quantity of particles
LargeMediumSmall
Size of particlesLargeA1A2A3
MediumA4
SmallA5
Coating material TYPE-B
Quantity of particles
LargeMediumSmall
Size of particlesLargeB1B2B3
MediumB4
SmallB5
Figure 3.
Examples of pictures of sample surfaces: (a) samples painted with TYPE-A; (b) samples painted with TYPE-B.
Participants were presented each sample separately and evaluated how strong they felt each impression, which is extracted by a rating word distance measurement test from crinkle-finished samples on a five-level scale from “not felt at all” to “strongly felt.” They were encouraged to clamp samples and view them at various angles. They were encouraged to evaluate only visual impressions, not haptic impressions. The samples were shown in random order. Eleven participants evaluated low-level impressions. Among 11 participants, 10 participants had also evaluated low-level impressions in previous experiments and 1 was a new participant. Ten participants evaluated high-level impressions. They had also evaluated high-level impressions in previous experiments.
2.2
Results
To extract the principal impressions, the average subjective scores were calculated and factor analyses of the low- and high-level impressions were performed. The maximum-likelihood method was used to extract low-level impressions. The least-squares method was used to extract high-level impressions because factor analysis did not converge using the maximum-likelihood method. Promax rotation was used for both analyses. Table III shows the factor loadings of the evaluation words for low-level impressions. The cumulative contribution ratio up to the second factor is 76.07%.
Table III.
Results of factor analysis for low-level impressions.
The results reveal two factors for low-level impressions. The first consists of the words “lumpy,” “rough,” “granular,” “glittering,” and “smooth.” The second consists of the words “matte,” “cloudy,” and “fine.”
We define the first factor as “macroroughness” because it corresponds to a large-scale impression regarding the particles of the crinkle finishes. The second factor is defined as “microroughness” because it corresponds to a smaller-scale impression.
Subjective scores of low-level impressions were calculated by factor analysis. Figure 4 shows the subjective scores of low-level impressions for each sample. This result indicates that macroroughness becomes higher as the quantity of particles becomes larger. Microroughness becomes higher as the size of the particles becomes smaller. The type of paint does not relate to these two impressions.
Figure 4.
Subjective scores of low-level impressions for each sample. The blue marker indicates the samples with coating material TYPE-A and the red marker indicates TYPE-B.
Table IV shows the factor loadings of each evaluation word for high-level impressions. The factor analysis was performed using nine evaluation words (except “slick”) because the number of evaluation words must be smaller than the number of samples. The cumulative contribution ratio up to the third factor is 81.40%.
Table IV.
Results of factor analysis for high-level impressions.
There are three factors for high-level impressions. The first consists of the words “sparkling,” “nonuniform,” “bumpy,” “lumpy,” “clear,” “feels premium,” and “simple.” We define this factor as “randomness” because the first factor is positively correlated with “nonuniform” and “sparkling” and negatively correlated with “simple.” The second and third factors consist of the words “stately” and “fine,” respectively, and hence we call these factors “stateliness” and “fineness.”
Subjective scores of high-level impressions were calculated by factor analysis, and Figure 5 shows that for each sample. This result indicates that randomness increases with increasing size of particles. As regards high-level impressions, there is no difference among the types of paints. The quantity of particles does not show a simple relation to high-level impressions.
Figure 5.
Subjective scores of high-level impressions for each sample. The blue marker indicates the samples with coating material TYPE-A and the red marker indicates TYPE-B.
3.
Experiment 2
We modeled the relationships among the five impression factors and the characteristics of crinkle-finished surface images. We measured the image characteristics using the processes described below and modeled the relationship using multiple regression analyses.
3.1
Methods
Figure 6 shows a schematic diagram of the multi-angle measurement system that we constructed to collect images of crinkle-finished samples. The system can measure the samples at various lighting and camera angles. The lighting device is a Xe light source (MAX-303, Asahi Spectra Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). The light beam is parallelized by a rod lens and a telecentric lens. The camera device is an XY Z camera (RTC-21, Ikegami Tsuhinki Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). The XY Z camera measures XY Z tristimulus values as a pixel value [8]. In this experiment, the resolution of the test images is 40 pixels/mm. The lighting and camera angles are defined as angles from the line that is normal to the measuring surface.
Figure 6.
Multi-angle measurement system.
We measured the paint surfaces of the crinkle-finished samples under eight conditions (Figure 7). Table V shows the size of the region of interest (ROI) under each condition. In this report, the measurement conditions are written as “lighting angle [deg]/camera angle [deg].”
Figure 7.
Conditions of the test images.
Table V.
ROI size for each condition.
Camera angleSize of ROISize of ROI
(deg)(pixel)(mm)
0600 × 60015.0 × 15.0
45424 × 42410.4 × 10.4
60300 × 3007.5 × 7.5
Figure 8 shows example test images. Figs.  8(a), (b), and (c) are images of sample A1 under three different conditions (450, 3045, and 060, respectively). These three images were measured at the same position. Here, (c) is part of a region of (b) and (b) is part of a region of (a). The sizes of ROIs were changed due to changing camera angles (shown in Table V). The brightness of particles changes in Fig. 8(a) and (b) as the red circled areas show. In Figs. 8(a) and (c), there are bright points that are smaller than the particles (circled in green).
Figure 8.
The results of measurement images.
We calculated the image characteristics using the following methods. First, we measured the Y-channel images of the samples Y (i, j) and a standard white target Y s(i, j) using the XY Z camera under each condition, where (i, j) denote spatial coordinates. Then, we calculated the brightness L of each sample to consider human visual characteristics. The L images of samples L(i, j) were calculated as defined by the International Commission on Illumination as follows:
(2)
L(i,j)=116fY(i,j)Ys(i,j)16.
Here, f(t) is defined as in the following equations:
(3)
f(t)t13t<(629)3[(293)3t+16]116t(629)3.
Finally, we calculated the following nine image characteristics:
(1) Average L
This characteristic is the average value of L in the ROI.
(2) Standard deviation ofL
This characteristic is the standard deviation of L in the ROI.
(3) Average Lof the base and particle regions
The L-channel images in the ROI were divided into base and particle regions using a threshold set for the average value of L in the samples. The average values of L in each region are defined as the average L values of the base and particle areas, respectively.
(4) Contrast ofL
This value is the absolute value of the difference between the average L values of the base and particles.
(5) Low and high spatial frequency characteristics
Figure 9 shows how the spatial frequency characteristics are calculated. As shown in Fig. 9(a), we calculated spatial frequency using the Fourier transformation of the test images and transformed the results into one-dimensional values by averaging the spatial frequency concentrically. As shown in Fig. 9(b), the integral of the one-dimensional spatial frequency values over a frequency range (0.5–2.0 and 4.0–10 cycles/mm for low and high spatial frequencies, respectively) is used as a spatial frequency characteristic [4]. The low spatial frequency characteristic corresponds to the size of the larger particles and the high spatial frequency characteristic corresponds to that of the smaller particles.
Figure 9.
(a) Procedure for calculating frequency characteristics. (b) Ranges of low- and high-frequency characteristics.
(6) Number of particles
We binarized the L-channel images using a threshold to divide them into particle and base regions. Then, we calculated the number of particles in the image.
(7) Average particle area
The average particle area was calculated from each connected component of the binarized images.
(8) L change at 45 and 60
The difference of the average L under highlighted (specular reflection) conditions and shaded (diffuse reflection) conditions is defined as the L change. The L change at 45 is the difference between 4545 (specular) and 045 (diffuse) images. The L change at 60 is the difference between 6060 (specular) and 060 (diffuse) images.
(9) Particle Lchange
The difference in L values for the particle regions between near-specular and diffuse conditions is defined as the particle L change. The difference between 3045 and 045 was calculated.
Image characteristics 1–9 were calculated for each of the eight test conditions (see Fig. 4). Therefore, a total of 75 image characteristics were calculated for each sample.
We derived formulas to predict the five impression factors using multiple regression analyses with a stepwise method. In these analyses, the objective variables are the factor scores of the five factors and the explanatory variables are the image characteristics. The values of the image characteristics have dissimilar ranges. Therefore, the standardized Z scores of the image characteristics were used as the explanatory variables.
3.2
Results
3.2.1
Macroroughness
Macroroughness is estimated as follows:
(4)
Macroroughness =0.978×x11.
Here, x11 is the 4545 high spatial frequency characteristic. Figure 10 shows the relationship between the values predicted from Eq. (4) and the subjective scores for macroroughness obtained by the impression evaluation experiments.
Figure 10.
Predicted values and subjective scores for macroroughness.
Figure 11.
Predicted values and subjective scores for microroughness.
Equation (4) indicates that macroroughness is highly positively correlated with the 4545 high spatial frequency characteristic. The R-squared value of Eq. (4) is 0.95. By k-fold cross-validation (k = 11), a mean R-squared value of 0.924 was obtained.
3.2.2
Microroughness
Microroughness is estimated as follows:
(5)
Microroughness =0.529×x210.51×x221.03×x23.
Here, x21 is the 450 standard deviation of L, x22 is the 6045 base value of L, and x23 is the 045 average particle area. Figure 11 shows the relationship between the values predicted from Eq. (5) and the subjective scores.
Equation (5) indicates that microroughness is positively correlated with the standard deviation of L and negatively correlated with the base value of L values and the average particle area. The R-squared value is 0.863. By k-fold cross-validation (k = 11), a mean R-squared value of 0.664 was obtained.
3.2.3
Randomness
The following equation estimates randomness:
(6)
Randomness =0.976×x31.
Here, x31 is the 4530 average particle area. Figure 12 shows the relationship between the values predicted from (Eq. (6)) and the subjective scores.
Equation (6) indicates that randomness is positively correlated with particle areas. The R-squared value is 0.961. By k-fold cross-validation (k = 10), a mean R-squared value of 0.937 was obtained.
Figure 12.
Predicted values and subjective scores for randomness.
3.2.4
Stateliness
Stateliness can be estimated as follows:
(7)
Stateliness =0.701×x41+0.771×x420.811×x43.
Here, x41 is the 450 low spatial frequency characteristic, x42 is the 3060 standard deviation of L, and x43 is the 6060 standard deviation of L. Figure 13 shows the relationship between the values predicted from Eq. (7) and the subjective scores.
Figure 13.
Predicted values and subjective scores for stateliness.
Equation (7) indicates that stateliness is positively correlated with low spatial frequency characteristics and the 3060 standard deviation of L. Furthermore, it is negatively correlated with the 6060 standard deviation of L. The R-squared value is 0.973. By k-fold cross-validation (k = 10), a mean R-squared value of 0.921 was obtained.
3.2.5
Fineness
Fineness is estimated as follows:
(8)
Fineness =1.817×x511.582×x521.676×x53.
Here, x51 is the 045 contrast of L, x52 is the 045 standard deviation of L, and x53 is the 060 contrast of L. Figure 14 shows the relationship between the values predicted from Eq. (8) and the subjective scores.
Figure 14.
Predicted values and subjective scores for fineness.
Equation (8) indicates that fineness is positively correlated with the 045 contrast of L. Furthermore, it is negatively correlated with the 045 standard deviation of L and the 060 contrast of L. The R-squared value is 0.957. By k-fold cross-validation (k = 10), a mean R-squared value of 0.937 was obtained.
4.
Discussion
A crinkle finish is usually used on digital cameras to provide a premium feeling. However, the results of the impression-word appropriateness test did not include “feels premium” as an appropriate term for crinkle-finished surfaces. Hence, subjects are not given much of a premium-feeling impression from a limited observation area of a planar part. This indicates that the premium feeling of a digital camera is affected by the overall shape of the camera.
Macroroughness is positively correlated with the high spatial frequency characteristic of the finish under specular conditions. This indicates that users perceive the luminance changes at the spatial frequency corresponding to the size of particles as macroroughness.
Microroughness is positively correlated with the variation of L and negatively correlated with the value of L in the base and particle areas. This indicates that users feel microroughness strongly when there are large luminance changes, a dark base, and small particles.
Randomness is positively correlated with the particle area. This result indicates that crinkle-finished surfaces with large particles invoke a strong feeling of randomness.
Stateliness is positively correlated with the low spatial frequency characteristic and the variation of L under diffuse conditions and negatively correlated with L variation under specular conditions. This indicates that large particles invoke a strong feeling of stateliness in users. Furthermore, users feel stateliness strongly when they see many bright points under diffuse conditions and view uniform brightness under specular conditions.
Fineness is positively correlated with the 045L contrast and negatively correlated with the 045 variation and the 060 contrast of L. We believe that the 045 contrast corresponds to the impression of the particles. However, the 045 variation and the 060 contrast of L correspond to the glitter of tiny light spots. Therefore, the results of this study show that users feel fineness strongly when the impression of particles at 045 is strong and the glitter of particles at 045 and 060 is low.
5.
Conclusion
We conducted an impression-word extraction test, impression-word appropriateness test, and rating word distance measurement test to extract evaluation words that represent the impressions of crinkle-finished surfaces.
The results of the impression evaluation tests indicate that low-level impressions consist of macroroughness and microroughness. High-level impressions consist of randomness, stateliness, and fineness. Furthermore, the words “feels premium” were not chosen as an evaluation term in the impression-word appropriateness tests.
Equations to predict five impression factors from test image characteristics were derived using multiple regression analyses. These prediction models indicate that impression factors are affected by not only simple particle parameters like quantity and size but also appearances at various lighting and observation angles. These proposed models enable attractive crinkle-finished surfaces to be designed. They further prevent designers from designing crinkle finishes with unintended or negative impressions. These models can improve the affective value of DSLR cameras.
In future, we will model the relationship between low-level and high-level impressions. Furthermore, we will analyze the impressions of the overall appearance of a DSLR camera, including paints and shapes.
Acknowledgment
This research is supported by the Center of Innovation Program from Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST).
References
1ChangH.HuangK.ChenH.HuangC.2018Evaluation of packaging for m regarding consumers’ sentimental response to bottled beverage containersAppl. Syst. Innov.11610.3390/asi1020016
2ChuC.HuangS.WangI.LoC.2013“Evaluating interactions between appearance-related product designs and facial characteristics,”2013 Fifth Int’l. Conf. on Service Science and Innovation (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2013)
3GotohT.SoneT.TaniY.TobitaniK.NagataN.Impression evaluation of camera exterior painted by chirimen patternThe 19th Annual Meeting of Japan Society of Kansei Engineering2017Japan Society of Kansei EngineeringTokyo, Japan
4HiroseY.1988Image noise evaluation method for xerographic prints of digitized imageJapan Hardcopy ’88189
5KatahiraK.MutoK.HashimotoS.TobitaniK.NagataN.2018The hierarchical approach to the semantic differential method: - The equivocality of “evaluation” factor in the EPA structureTrans. Japan Soc. Kansei Eng.1745310.5057/jjske.TJSKE-D-17-00075
6KoishiS.NonakaH.KuriharaM.2008Extraction and application of human individuality in colors based on hierarchy of Kansei wordsJ. Japan Soc. Fuzzy Theory Intelligent Inform.2014110.3156/jsoft.20.141
7KouS.MoriyamaN.KawasumiM.2018Comparative study on visual impression structure for car front grill between Japan and Thailand7th Int’l. Conf. on Kansei Engineering & Emotion Research 2018151156151–6Springer SingaporeBeach Rd, Singapore
8KretkowskiM.JablonskiR.ShimodairaY.2010Development of an XYZ digital camera with embedded color calibration system for accurate color acquisitionIEICE Trans. Inform Syst.E93-D65110.1587/transinf.E93.D.651
9MakikoS.WadaY.2012Individual difference analysis in impression evaluation of the appearance colors of cellular phonesTrans. Japan Soc. Kansei Eng.114710.5057/jjske.11.47
10MeiY.LebinJ.“Study on Kansei engineering and its application to product design,” 2009 Second Int’l. Symposium on Computational Intelligence and Design (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2009
11MunechikaM.MiwaT.2000A guideline for selection of evaluation words used in questionnaire of Kansei qualityJ. Japan. Soc. Qual. Control3096
12LeeK. N. N.TobitaniK.KatahiraK.NagataN.ShiraiwaA.NikataK.ArakawaK.IshiiK.TsukiyamaF.Modeling of ‘high-class feeling’ on a cosmetic package designThe 22nd Korea-Japan Joint Workshop on Frontiers of Computer Vision (FCV2016)2016IEEJTokyo, Japan
13NakamuraM.NakagawaS.NakanoT.2015Evaluation of visual impact of multiple image characteristics observed in edge-grain patternsJ. Wood Sci.611910.1007/s10086-014-1439-6
14OtaY.HigashiH.NakauchiS.“Objective assessment and quantification of pearl quality by spectral-spatial features,” 2nd Int’l. Conf. on Advanced Informatics: Concepts, Theory and Applications (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2015)
15ParkL.TobitaniK.KatahiraK.NagataN.“Analysis of BRDF/BTDF for the texture representation of woven fabrics based on the impression-evaluation model,” 21st Korea-Japan Joint Workshop on Frontiers of Computer Vision (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2015)
16ShenH. C.WangK. C.2016“The application of Kansei engineering and SDM based creativity to product form design,” 2016 Int’l. Conf. on Applied System Innovation (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2016)
17TaniY.NagaiT.KoidaK.KitazakiM.NakauchiS.2014Experts and novices use the same factors—but differently—to evaluate pearl qualityPLoS ONE9e8640010.1371/journal.pone.0086400
18TobitaniK.NakajimaK.KatahiraK.NishijimaK.NagataN.Visibility study on design pattern of car tail lamp using perceptual sensitivity on face recognition abilitiesThe 22nd Korea-Japan Joint Workshop on Frontiers of Computer Vision (FCV2016)2016IEEJTokyo, Japan
19ToyotaT.NakauchiS.2013Optical measurement of interference color of pearls and its relation to subjective qualityOpt. Rev.205010.1007/s10043-013-0009-2
20Xing-yuanW.XuY.“Study on consumer’s Kansei image evaluation for high-tech consumable products,” 2006 Int’l. Conf. on Management Science and Engineering (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2006)
21YamadaA.HashimotoS.NagataN.2018Automatic impression indexing based on evaluate expression dictionary from review dataTrans. Japan Soc. Kansei Eng.1756710.5057/jjske.TJSKE-D-18-00065